Cognición Esquizotipia y Esquizofrenia

download Cognición Esquizotipia y Esquizofrenia

of 5

Transcript of Cognición Esquizotipia y Esquizofrenia

  • 8/12/2019 Cognicin Esquizotipia y Esquizofrenia

    1/5

    Aspects of cognitive functioning in schizotypy and schizophrenia: Evidence for a

    continuum model

    Mary Cochrane a,, Ian Petch b, Alan D. Pickering c,1

    a Division of Psychological Medicine and Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, 4 Windsor Walk, London SE5 8BB, United Kingdomb Early Intervention Team, Springeld University Hospital, 61 Glenburnie Road, London SW17 7DJ, United Kingdomc Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths College, University of London, SE14 6NW, United Kingdom

    a b s t r a c ta r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:Received 27 August 2011

    Received in revised form 7 February 2012

    Accepted 8 February 2012

    Keywords:

    Schizotypy

    Schizophrenia

    Cognitive functioning

    Continuum approach

    This research consisted of two studies, the fundamental aim of which was to delineate the pattern of relationshipsbetween measures of cognitive task performance and both symptom subtypes in schizophrenia and their

    corresponding schizotypal personality traits in healthy individuals. Study 1 compared these relationships in healthy

    individuals using the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) and Study 2 assessed the relationships between

    symptomatology assessedusing the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms and Scale for the Assessment of

    Negative Symptoms (SAPS/SANS) and cognitive task performance in a group of patients with schizophrenia. The

    contribution ofuid intelligence to task performance was also examined. In Study 1 high levels of negative schizo-

    typy were associated with reduced verbaluency, and high levels of disorganised schizotypy were associated with

    reduced negative priming in the healthy participants. In Study 2, closely corresponding relationships between

    symptom measures and these tasks were found in the patients with schizophrenia. The associations between

    the symptom and cognitive measures were independent of the effects ofuid IQ on performance.

    2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

    1. Introduction

    A growing body of research suggests that symptoms of schizo-

    phrenia and corresponding elevated schizotypal personality scores

    in non-clinical samples are associated with similar disruptions in cog-

    nitive functioning. This accords with the fully dimensional approach

    to schizophrenia, which proposes that the dimension of schizotypy

    lies on a continuum that begins with normality and proceeds towards

    the schizophrenia spectrum disorders, with schizophrenia at the

    upper end (Claridge and Beech, 1995).

    Verbal uency (VF)and negative priming (NP) are two cognitive phe-

    notypes which are consistently found to be impaired in patients with

    schizophrenia and in healthy individuals scoring high on measures of

    schizotypal personality.However, relationshipsbetween aspectsof symp-

    tomatology and cognitive performance are not always consistent and, in

    the case of negative priming, there is limited research assessing the way

    in which differential aspects of symptomatology relate to performance.

    Verbal uency is a commontest of response generation, in which par-

    ticipants are asked to generate, in a one-minute interval, as many words

    as possiblethat begin witheach of three letters.Patients withschizophre-

    niatypically generatefewer words thancontrols on VF tasks andnegative

    symptoms are associated with these decits (Liddle and Morris, 1991;

    Frith, 1992). Negative symptomratings are also correlated withimpairedVF in patients with schizotypal personality disorder (Diforio et al., 2000),

    and negative schizotypy is positively associated with VF decits in the

    healthy siblings of schizophrenic patients (Franke et al., 1993).

    Reported relationships between schizotypal personality traits and VF

    in healthy participants are somewhat inconsistent. Tsakanikos and

    Claridge (2005) found that reduced VF was associated with increased

    O-LIFE(Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences) negative

    schizotypy, but also found thatgreaterVF was associated with high levels

    of O-LIFE positive schizotypy. Relationships have been found between

    reduced VF and high scores on both negative and positive schizotypy

    (Barrantes-Vidal et al., 2003) and between reduced VF and high scores

    on both negative and disorganized schizotypy, or positive schizotypy

    (Ruiz et al., 2008). These latter studies employed a cluster-analytic rather

    than a dimensional approach to schizotypal personality, which may (at

    least partially) explain the differential ndings.

    The NP paradigm is believed to assess selective attention and cog-

    nitive inhibition (Tipper, 1985). Reduced negative priming has been

    found in patients with schizophrenia (Beech et al., 1989; Park et al.,

    1996; Williams, 1996; Peters et al., 2000), although some authors

    have not observed NP decits in patients (Laplante et al., 1992;

    Baving et al., 2001). A possible explanation for this discrepancy is pro-

    vided byMoritz et al. (2001).There is a striking paucity of research

    assessing relationships between aspects of symptomatology and neg-

    ative priming.Williams (1996)reported an association between both

    positive and disorganised symptomatology and reduced NP in schizo-

    phrenia, although this nding is yet to be replicated. Impaired NP in

    Psychiatry Research 196 (2012) 230234

    Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 207 848 0656.

    E-mail addresses:[email protected](M. Cochrane),

    [email protected](I. Petch),[email protected](A.D. Pickering).1 Tel.: +20 7919 7334.

    0165-1781/$ see front matter 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

    doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.02.010

    Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

    Psychiatry Research

    j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / p s y c h r e s

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.02.010http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.02.010http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.02.010mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.02.010http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/01651781http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/01651781http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.02.010mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.02.010http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/12/2019 Cognicin Esquizotipia y Esquizofrenia

    2/5

    healthy participants appears to be primarily associated with positive

    schizotypy (Peters et al., 1994; Park et al., 1996).

    The current research consisted of two studies, which attempted to

    ascertain whether relationships between aspects of symptomatology/

    schizotypal personality and performance on two popular cognitive

    tasks would be consistent across a group of healthy participants and

    a group of patients with schizophrenia. A consistent patternof relation-

    ships would provide convincing evidence for the continuum approach

    to schizophrenia and uniquely demonstrate that both clinical symp-toms and non-clinical characteristics impact in a similar way on cogni-

    tive functioning. A measure of uid IQ was included to investigate

    whether any relationships between symptomatology and cognition

    were maintained despite differences in a general cognitive ability.

    In the healthy participants in Study 1, it was expected that higher

    levels of SPQ (Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire) negative schizo-

    typy would be associated with reduced verbaluency,and that higher

    levels of SPQ positive or disorganised schizotypy would be associated

    with reduced negative priming. In the patient group we expected the

    same pattern of relationships between symptomatology and cognitive

    task performance, using the Scale for the Assessment of Positive

    Symptoms/Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SAPS/

    SANS) inventory (Andreasen, 1983, 1984). Finally, it was assumed

    that these effects would be independent of the expected positive rela-

    tionships between uid IQ and the cognitive measures.

    2. Method

    2.1. Ethical approval

    Ethical approval for Studies 1 and 2 was obtained from the Psychology Department

    Ethics Committee at Goldsmiths College. For Study 2, ethical approval was also

    obtained from the South West London NHS Local Research Ethics Committee.

    2.2. Participants

    Study 1 consisted of a convenience sample of 100 participants (50 females and 50

    males), recruited from the student population at Goldsmiths College, University of

    London. Participants were recruited by means of advertisements posted around the

    college and received either course credits (psychology students) or a small remunera-

    tion for their participation. Age range was between 18 and 45 years (M=25.00,

    S.D.=6.09). Seventy one percent of 99 participants were native English speakers (lan-

    guage information was not available for one participant), although all participants

    were uent in English. The mean number of years spent in education was

    16.17 years (S.D.= 2.85). Exclusion criteria, determined through self-report, were pre-

    vious or current psychological or neurological disturbance, a serious physical medical

    condition, or currently taking psychoactive medication.

    Twenty patients with schizophrenia took part in Study 2: 10 were inpatients and 10

    were living in the community. There were 18 males and 2 females. All patients had an

    ICD-10 diagnosis of schizophrenia, were taking atypical neuroleptic medication, and

    were uent in English. Age range was between 18 and 50 years (M=34.24,

    S.D.=8.42). Patients with a diagnosed substance abuse disorder, concomitant neurolog-

    ical disorder or serious physical medical condition were excluded from the study. The in-

    patients were recruited from three wards at Springeld University Hospital, South West

    London, and were tested in a quiet consulting room on the ward. The community pa-

    tients were recruited from three Community Mental Health Teams in the South West

    London area and were tested at an outpatient clinic that they regularly attended.

    2.3. Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ, Raine, 1991)

    Study 1

    The SPQ is a forced choice (Yes/No) self-report inventory which contains 74 items

    and divides into nine subscales based on the nine criteria for DSM-III-R schizotypal per-

    sonality disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 1987). These subscales are Ideas of

    Reference (IoR), Unusual Perceptual Experiences (UPE), Odd Beliefs or Magical Thinking

    (OBMT), Paranoid Ideation (PI), Excessive Social Anxiety (ESA), No Close Friends (NCF),

    Constricted Affect (CA), Odd or Eccentric Behaviour (OEB), and Odd Speech (OS). The

    SPQ also sub-divides into three factors: CognitivePerceptual, Interpersonal and Disorga-

    nised;whichare regarded as non-clinical analoguesof thepositive, negative anddisorga-

    nised symptoms of schizophrenia respectively (Raine et al., 1991).

    2.4. Clinical Interview Schedule (SAPS/SANS)Study 2

    The SAPS/SANS consist of a total of 51 items, with each item scored between 0 (ab-

    sence of a symptom) to 5 (severe/complex symptomatology). The items fall into nine

    global subscales, and the scores on these subscales are summed to yield three factors;

    positive, negative and disorganised. Two attentional items were excluded from the

    factor score calculations as their scores do not load consistently on any of the three fac-

    tors (Andreasen et al., 1995and personal correspondence with Andreasen).

    2.5. Measures of cognitive functioning anduid IQ

    Verbal uency was assessed using the Controlled Oral Word Association Test

    (COWAT,Benton and Hamsher, 1976), in which participants are asked to produce as

    many words as possible that begin with each of the letters F, A and S (with one minute

    for each letter); without using proper nouns, plural words, or the same word with dif-

    ferent sufxes. The dependent variable was the total number of correct words generat-

    ed in response to the three letters.

    The computerised spatial negative priming task was based on the paradigm used by

    Park et al. (1996). Displays contained a target (letter O) and a distractor (letter X), and

    there were four possible locations on the screen in which the target or distractor could ap-

    pear. Each trial consisted of a prime followed by a probe display. Participants were asked to

    respond only to the letter O and to ignore the X. In control trials, the locations of the targets

    anddistractors in both prime andprobe displayswere different. In NP trials, thetarget in the

    probe display appeared in the same position as the distractor in the prime display, and thus

    participants were required to respond to the previously ignored location. The overlap be-

    tween the target and distractor in the NP trials is believed to produce response retardation

    since the active inhibition of ignored information during the prime display may decrease its

    accessibility for later selection during the probe display (Tipper, 1985).

    Participants focused on a central xation point and responded to the location of the

    targetO as quickly andas accurately as possible. Responseswere made by pressing oneof

    four keys on a special response box attached to the computer. Trials were initiated by

    pressing the spacebar and each trial began with the presentation of the centralxation

    point for 0.8 s. Theprime display was then presenteduntil participants located the target

    and responded. After a further 0.5 s the central xation point appeared (again for 0.8 s)followed by the probe display which remained on the screen until a response was

    made. After participants responded to the probe display, a pattern mask appeared for

    1 s and then the next trial began. There were four blocks of 18 pairs of experimental tri-

    als, consistingof 9 NPand 9 control trials, whichwere presented ina xed random order.

    A set of 10 practice trials preceded the experimental trials.

    The main dependent measure was the NP reaction time score (NPrt), which was

    calculated by subtracting the mean reaction time for control probe trials from the

    mean reaction time for NP probe trials. Separate mean reaction time scores for the

    rst and second blocks of the task (n =36 trials in each block) were also computed

    to ascertain whether any decits in performance and their corresponding schizotypal

    features were specic to the initial or the latter phase of the task.

    The matrices from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III (Wechsler, 1997) were

    used as a brief measure of uid intelligence. In this untimed test participants were

    shown a series of 26 matrices, each of which had a missing cell. For each item partici-

    pants were asked to choose the correct completion from 5 options, based on the rela-

    tionships (across rows and columns, etc.) for the other cells in the matrix. An initial

    raw score was calculated by summing the number of correct items completed by par-ticipants (with a maximum score of 26) and the raw scores were then converted to

    age-adjusted scaled scores as described in the manual.

    2.6. Procedure

    All participants were asked to read and sign a consent form and to provide the de-

    mographic information: age, gender, number of years in education, and whether En-

    glish was their native language (a binary variable coded 1= native English,

    0= English not native language). In Study 1 the healthy participants were then

    asked to complete the SPQ and cognitive and uid IQ tasks. One participant failed to

    complete both of the cognitive tasks and two failed to complete the negative priming

    task; therefore, these participants were excluded from the analyses.

    In Study 2 the patientsrecords were accessed to obtain information on diagnosis,

    medication and general medical history. Patients completed the SAPS/SANS interview,

    followed by the cognitive tasks and WAIS-III matrices. In each of the two studies half of

    the participants performed the uid IQ test before the cognitive tests and the remain-

    ing participants performed the IQ test afterwards. Systematic order effects were mini-mised by also counterbalancing the order of presentation of the verbal uency and NP

    tasks. On completion of the tasks all participants were debriefed, provided with a small

    remuneration, and thanked for their participation.

    3. Results

    3.1. Study 1healthy participants

    3.1.1. Data distributions and outlier removal

    Scores on the SPQ and verbal uency measures were normally dis-

    tributed. One outlier score was found in the overall NP index and one

    in the Block 1 NP index, and two outliers were present in the Block 2

    NP index. These scores were removed in order to normalise the data

    distributions.

    231M. Cochrane et al. / Psychiatry Research 196 (2012) 230234

  • 8/12/2019 Cognicin Esquizotipia y Esquizofrenia

    3/5

    3.1.2. Descriptive statistics

    Descriptives for the SPQ, cognitive and uid IQ measures are pre-

    sented inTable 1. The SPQ positive factor was composed of the Unusual

    PerceptualExperiences, Ideasof Reference, Odd Beliefs/Magical Thinking

    and Paranoid Ideation scores, and the disorganised factor comprised the

    summation of the Odd Speech and Odd/Eccentric Behaviour scores. The

    negative factor was composed of the Excessive Social Anxiety, No Close

    Friends and Constricted Affect scores. The internal consistency of the

    SPQ subscales was assessed by coefcient alpha and the values accorded

    closely with Raine's (1991) coefcients, which ranged from 0.71 to 0.78.

    The mean SPQ total score of 25.0 (S.D.=13.4) was consistent with the

    mean SPQ total scores of two groups of healthy participants reported

    by Raine et al. (1991): 26.9 (S.D.= 11.0) and 26.3 (S.D.=11.4) in

    Samples 1 and 2 respectively.

    3.1.3. Correlational analyses

    Bonferroni adjustments were made for multiple correlations be-

    tween the personality measures, whereas the exploratory correla-

    tions between uid IQ and the schizotypal personality measures,

    and between the demographic and cognitive measures, were tested

    at an unadjusted alpha ofP0.05. Correlations between uid IQ and

    the cognitive measures were tested one-tailed, as we expected that

    higher levels ofuid IQ would be associated with enhanced perfor-

    mance (higher VF, increased NP) on the cognitive tasks.

    3.1.3.1. Intercorrelations among the SPQ factors.The intercorrelations of

    the main personality variables were assessed using 3 Pearson's

    correlations and were therefore tested at an adjusted alpha of

    P0.017 (~0.05/3). There was a signicant correlation between the

    CognitivePerceptual factor and the Interpersonal factor (r=0.53,

    Pb0.001) and between the CognitivePerceptual and Disorganised

    factors (r=0.59, Pb0.001). There was also a signicant correlation

    between the Interpersonal and Disorganised factors (r=0.35, Pb0.001).

    3.1.3.2. Fluid IQ and schizotypy.There were no signicant relation-

    ships between

    uid IQ and the SPQ positive (r=

    0.11), negative(r=0.10) and disorganised (r=0.08) factors (P>0.05).

    3.1.3.3. Fluid IQ and cognitive task performance.Fluid IQ was signi-

    cantly positively associated with verbal uency (r=0.42, Pb0.001)

    and with performance in Block 1 of the NP task ( r=0.18, P=0.04,

    1-tailed). However, there was no signicant correlation between

    uid IQ and the overall NP index (r=0.06), or the Block 1 NP

    index (r=0.03) (P>0.05).

    3.1.3.4. Demographic variables and cognitive task performance. There

    were no signicant relationships between the demographic and

    cognitive measures (P>0.05).

    3.2. Multiple regression analyses

    Four standard multiple regressions were conducted to assess the

    independent contribution of the personality measures to the predic-

    tion of each cognitive (dependent) measure. Fluid IQ was added to

    the regressions assessing VF and Block 1 NP, as was the language var-

    iable, which was signicantly positively correlated with VF (r=0.37,

    Pb0.001) and Block 1 NP (r=0.21, Pb0.05). Negative and disorga-

    nised schizotypal features as predictors of VF and NP respectively

    were tested one-tailed, given the direction of the effects reported in

    previous research. The independent contribution of uid IQ to the

    prediction of cognitive task performance was also tested one-tailed.

    Positive schizotypal features were tested as predictors two-tailed

    given the occasional report of an effect in the opposite direction to

    the majority of effects (Tsakanikos and Claridge, 2005). The resultsof the regression analyses are shown inTable 2.

    3.2.1. Verbal uency

    The SPQ factors, uid IQ and language were used as predictors,

    with the number of words generated in the VF task as the dependent

    variable. There was a near-signicant trend for a negative effect of

    SPQ negative schizotypy (=0.17, Pb0.06, one-tailed), over and

    above the robustly signicant independent contributions ofuid IQ

    (=0.33,Pb0.001) and English as rst language (=0.34,Pb0.001).

    3.2.2. Negative priming (NP)

    Regression analysis with the SPQ factors as predictors of the over-

    all NP reaction time index revealed that higher levels of disorganised

    Table 1

    Descriptives for the personality and cognitive measuresStudy 1.

    Mean (S.D.) Min Max N Alpha

    SPQ factors

    Positive (cognitiveper ceptual) 11.14 ( 7.15) 0.00 30.00 99 0.89

    Negati ve (interpersonal) 6.98 (5.08) 0.00 22.00 100 0.86

    Disorganisation 7.00 (4.07) 0.00 14.00 99 0.85

    SPQ subscales

    Ideas of reference 3.94 (2.60) 0.00 9.00 100 0.77Odd beliefs/magical t hinking 2.26 ( 2.18) 0.00 7.00 100 0.81

    Unusual perceptual experiences 2.49 (2.36) 0.00 9.00 99 0.77

    Paranoid ideation 2.55 (2.26) 0.00 8.00 100 0.78

    Excessi ve social anxiety 3.00 (2.29) 0.00 8.00 100 0.77

    No close friends 2.06 (2.03) 0.00 9.00 100 0.72

    Constricted affect 1.92 (1.65) 0.00 6.00 100 0.61

    Odd or eccentric behavior 2.60 (2.19) 0.00 7.00 99 0.82

    Odd speech 4.141 (2.49) 0.00 8.00 100 0.76

    Cognitive measures

    Verbaluency 40.78 (11.29) 13.00 73.00 99

    Negative priming (NP) 0.02 (0.05) 0.27 0.08 98

    Block 1 NP 0.00 (0.05) 0.19 0.13 97

    Block 2 NP 0.01 (0.04) 0.12 0.11 96

    Fluid IQ 14.10 (2.15) 9.00 19.00 99

    Table 2

    Predictors of executive functioningStudy 1.

    Dependent measures

    Verbaluency Negative priming Block 1 negative priming Block 2 negative priming

    Predictors B SEB B SEB B SEB B SEB

    Fluid IQ 1.752 0.473 0.334*** 0.004 0.002 0.173*

    Language 8.405 2.238 0.341*** 0.020 0.011 0.182

    SPQ pos 0.069 0.197 0.043 0.001 0.001 0.247 0.001 0.001 0.162 0.002 0.001 0.251

    SPQ neg 0.373 0.233 0.167 0.001 0.001 0.107 0.000 0.001 0.057 0.001 0.001 0.069

    SPQ disorg 0.021 0.308 0.008 0.002 0.001 0.244* 0.000 0.002 0.031 0.005 0.001 0.428***

    R2=0.296, adjR2=0.258

    (F(5, 92)=7.728, Pb0.001)

    R2=0.050, adjR2=0.020

    (F(3, 93)=1.647,ns)

    R2=0.084, adjR2=0.033

    (F(5, 90)=1.658,ns)

    R2=0.121, adjR2=0.093

    (F(3, 92)=4.236, Pb0.01)

    Note: SPQ Pos = SPQ CognitivePerceptual (positive) factor; SPQ Neg = SPQ Interpersonal (negative) factor; SPQ Disorg = SPQ Disorganisation factor.

    *P

    0.05, ***P

    0.001, trend effect (Pb

    0.06).

    232 M. Cochrane et al. / Psychiatry Research 196 (2012) 230234

  • 8/12/2019 Cognicin Esquizotipia y Esquizofrenia

    4/5

    schizotypy signicantly predicted reduced negative priming (=

    0.24,Pb0.03, 1-tailed). There was weak trend (=0.25,Pb0.08,

    2-tailed) for a positive association between negative priming per-

    formance and positive schizotypy, while the negative schizotypal

    factor did not signicantly contribute to the variance in NP.

    When the SPQ factors, uid IQ and language were used as predic-

    tors of the NP reaction time index in the rst block of the task,uid IQ

    was the only signicant predictor of (greater) NP (=0.17,P = 0.05,

    1-tailed). When the SPQ variables were used as predictors of NP per-formance in the second block of the task, positive schizotypy again

    tended to predict greater negative priming (=0.25, Pb0.07, 2-

    tailed); however, disorganised schizotypy independently and signi-

    cantly predicted reduced negative priming (=0.43,Pb0.001, 1-

    tailed).

    3.3. Study 2patients with schizophrenia

    3.3.1. Descriptive statistics

    Scores on the symptom and cognitive task measures were normal-

    ly distributed. The descriptives are shown inTable 3.

    3.3.2. Correlational analyses

    The analyses were conducted in accordance with Study 1. Pear-

    son's correlations used to assess the relationships between the symp-

    tom and demographic measures were tested two-tailed and were

    non-signicant (P>0.05). The correlations between uid IQ and the

    cognitive measures were tested one-tailed. There were no signicant

    correlations between the uid IQ measure and the negative priming

    indices; however, there was a trend for a positive relationship be-

    tweenuid IQ and VF (r=0.35,P=0.06).

    The relationships between the SAPS/SANS scores and cognitive

    task performance were tested one-tailed for negative and disorga-

    nised schizotypy and two-tailed for positive schizotypy. The negative

    factor scores were signicantly negatively associatedwithVF (r=0.43,

    P=0.03, 1-tailed) and the disorganised factor scores were signicantly

    negatively associated with NP (r=0.53,Pb0.01, 1-tailed). The signi-

    cant association between disorganised symptomatology and negative

    priming was specic to performance in the latter phase of the task(r=0.51, P=0.01, 1-tailed) as opposed to the initial phase (r=

    0.27, P=0.13, 1-tailed); the same pattern that was found in the

    healthy participants with SPQ disorganised schizotypy in Study 1.

    There were no signicant correlations between the positive factor

    scores and the cognitive measures.

    3.3.3. Multiple regression analysis

    Given the trend for a positive association between VF and uid IQ,

    and the robust and signicant positive correlation between these var-

    iables in the larger sample of healthy participants, VF was reanalysed

    in a regression using both SANS negative factor score and uid IQ as

    predictors. This reduced the independent prediction of VF by SANS

    negative factor score to a near-signicant trend (=0.36,Pb0.06,

    1-tailed), while the independent prediction of VF by uid IQ was

    non-signicant (=0.27, P=0.12, 1-tailed). (R2 =0.25, Adj. R2 =

    0.16,F(2, 17)=2.80,P>0.05).

    4. Discussion

    In accordance with expectations, high levels of negative schizo-

    typy predicted reduced verbal uency and high levels of SPQ disorga-

    nised schizotypy predicted reduced negative priming in the healthyparticipants in Study 1. This latter effect was particularly evident in

    the second block of the NP task. It seems that for participants high

    in disorganisation, inhibitory processing was initially intact, but a dif-

    culty in sustaining this ability was evident as the task progressed.

    Higher levels of SPQ positive schizotypy tended to predict enhanced

    inhibition in the latter part of the NP task; a nding which is at

    odds with other published NP results showing a negative relationship

    (Peters et al., 1994; Park et al., 1996) and therefore may require fur-

    ther investigation to determine if it is replicable. This direction of ef-

    fect has been previously reported for the inuence of positive

    schizotypy on VF (Tsakanikos and Claridge, 2005); however, no

    such effect was observed in the present study.

    Study 2 examined the pattern of relationships between clinical

    symptomatology and cognitive task performance in patients with

    schizophrenia and revealed the same pattern of relationships. Higher

    levels of disorganised symptoms were signicantly associated with

    reduced negative priming, and this relationship was specic to the

    second phase of the task. Higher levels of negative symptoms were

    signicantly correlated with reduced verbal uency; however, the

    prediction of VF by negative symptomatology was slightly reduced

    in a regression controlling for uid IQ. Nevertheless, on both tasks,

    continuity was observed across the groups of patients and healthy

    participants.

    The association between negative symptomatology and verbal u-

    ency is consistent with previous research in patient groups ( Liddle

    and Morris, 1991; Frith, 1992) and in schizotypal participants

    (Tsakanikos and Claridge, 2005). The relationship between SAPS dis-

    organisation and reduced NP in patients with schizophrenia corre-

    sponds with that reported by Williams (1996). However, theassociation between disorganisation and reduced NP in the schizoty-

    pal individuals does not accord with other reported relationships

    which have emphasised the link between positive schizotypy and re-

    duced NP. This discrepancy cannot be fully explained by the NP tasks

    used in the different studies:Peters et al. (1994)used a Stroop-based

    task but Park et al. (1996) used a spatial NP task, on which the current

    task was closely based. The inconsistency across studies could arise

    from the use of different schizotypy inventories: while Park et al.

    and Peters et al. used the Perceptual Aberration Scale (PAS,

    Chapman et al., 1978) and Combined Schizotypal Traits Questionnaire

    (CSTQ,Claridge et al., 1996) respectively, the current schizotypy/NP

    relationship was found using the SPQ. Interestingly, the relationship

    between disorganised schizophrenia and NP found in this study and

    byWilliams (1996)was obtained using the same instrument (SAPS/SANS). It may be difcult to clearly delineate the pattern of relation-

    ships between symptomatology and cognition when methods of

    assessing these phenomena are so varied.

    The inclusion of the uid IQ measure in these studies was impor-

    tant in highlighting the contribution of this process to cognitive task

    performance, and in demonstrating that uid IQ did not fully account

    for variations in performance. Fluid IQ may be regarded as a key com-

    ponent of cognitive functioning (Duncan, 1995) and yet the ability of

    schizophrenic symptomatology and schizotypal personality to predict

    residual variance in verbal uency, after accounting for the contribu-

    tion ofuid IQ, was established. Fluid IQ made no signicant contri-

    bution to the prediction of negative priming in the patients with

    schizophrenia, but did contribute to the variance in Block 1 NP in

    the healthy participants.

    Table 3

    Descriptives for the cognitive measures and uid IQStudy 2.

    Mean (S.D.) Min Max N

    Symptom measures (SAPS/SANS)

    Positive factor 5.05 (2.91) 0.00 9.00 20

    Negative factor 9.05 (3.58) 1.00 15.00 20

    Disorganised factor 1.90 (1.86) 0.00 6.00 20

    Cognitive measures

    Verbaluency 34.80 (8.57) 17.00 46.00 20

    Negative priming 0.01 (0.13) 0.47 0.26 20

    Block 1 NP 0.03 (0.12) 0.11 0.47 20

    Block 2 NP 0.04 (0.21) 0.91 0.76 20

    Fluid IQ 10.85 (2.28) 6.00 16.00 20

    233M. Cochrane et al. / Psychiatry Research 196 (2012) 230234

  • 8/12/2019 Cognicin Esquizotipia y Esquizofrenia

    5/5

    Our ndings are interpreted with some caution since we did not

    provide a formal assessment of some factors which may have impacted

    on the relationships observed between symptomatology and cognitive

    task performance. In Study 1 we did not have a formalised list of cri-

    teria for establishing a serious physical medical condition, nor did

    we perform any diagnostic assessment of psychopathology in the

    healthy participants; rather, in both instances, the data were obtained

    by means of self-report to general questions regarding these criteria.

    In the patient group in Study 2 we did not include an assessment ofthe class or dosage of atypical antipsychotic medication and therefore

    could not statistically assess potential effects of medication. Further-

    more, multiple regression analysis with SANS negative symptomatolo-

    gy anduid IQ as predictors of verbaluency was conducted with low

    power in such a small sample (n =20).

    It could be argued that the lack of a control group is also a limita-

    tion of Study 2. However, we do not regard this as a serious issue as

    the research here is concerned with the relationships between symp-

    toms and cognitive measures within a patient sample. The current re-

    search questions are largely unaffected by whether the schizophrenic

    group is signicantly impaired on a particular cognitive measure. If VF

    (or NP) relate to a specic class of symptoms, then whether the par-

    ticular schizophrenic sample, as a whole, is impaired with respect to

    an appropriate matched control group will depend on the level of

    the relevant symptom type: if it is high then the group will be im-

    paired as a whole, if it is low, then the group might well not show

    an impairment.

    In conclusion, this is the rst report of a consistent pattern of

    symptom-cognition relationships across a group of patients with

    schizophrenia and a large group of healthy participants. For these

    two specic relationships we thus provide convincing evidence for

    the continuum model; demonstrating not only that schizophrenia-

    like traits are present in the healthy population but also showing

    that these traits may lead to disruptions in cognitive performance in

    patients and schizotypal individuals in a similar manner. Future stud-

    ies may be further informed by the inclusion of multiple measures of

    cognition and additional culture-fair measures of general/uid

    intelligence.

    References

    American Psychiatric Association, 1987. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of MentalDisorders, 3rd edition revised. APA Press, Washington.

    Andreasen, N.C.,1983. The Scale for the Assessment of NegativeSymptoms (SANS). TheUniversity of Iowa, Iowa City.

    Andreasen, N.C., 1984. The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). TheUniversity of Iowa, Iowa City.

    Andreasen, N.C., Arndt, S., Alliger, R., Miller,D., Flaum, M., 1995. Symptoms of schizophrenia:methods, meanings and mechanisms. Archives of General Psychiatry 52, 341351.

    Barrantes-Vidal, N., Fananas, L., Rosa, A., Caparros, B., Riba, M.D., Obiols, J.E., 2003.Neurocognitive, behavioural and neurodevelopmental correlates of schizotypyclusters in adolescents from the general population. Schizophrenia Research 61,293302.

    Baving, L., Wagner, M., Cohen, R., Rockstroh, B., 2001. Increased repetition and semanticpriming in schizophrenic patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 110, 6775.

    Beech, A., Powell, T., McWilliam, J., Claridge, G., 1989. Evidence of reduced cognitiveinhibitionin schizophrenia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 28, 109116.

    Benton, A.L., Hamsher, K.deS, 1976. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. University ofIowa, Iowa City.

    Chapman, L.J., Chapman, J.P., Raulin, M.L., 1978. Body-image aberration in schizophrenia.

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology 86, 399407.Claridge, G., Beech, T., 1995. Fully and quasi-dimensional constructions of schizotypy.In: Raine, A., Lencz, T., Mednick, S.A. (Eds.), Schizotypal Personality. CambridgeUniversity Press, Cambridge, pp. 192216.

    Claridge, G., McCreery, C., Mason, O., Bentall, R., Boyle, G., Slade, P., Popplewell, D.,1996. The factor structure ofschizotypal traits: a large replication study. British

    Journal of Clinical Psychology 35, 103115.Diforio, D., Walker, E.F., Kestler, L.P., 2000. Executive functions in adolescents with

    schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophrenia Research 42, 125134.Duncan, J., 1995. Attention, intelligence, and the frontal lobes. In: Gazzaniga, M.S. (Ed.),

    The Cognitive Neurosciences. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 721733.Franke, P., Maier, W., Hardt, J., Hain, C., 1993. Cognitive functioning and anhedonia in

    subjects at risk for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 10, 7784.Frith, C.D., 1992. The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Psychology Press,

    East Sussex.Laplante, L., Everett, J., Thomas, J., 1992. Inhibition through negative priming with

    Stroop stimuli in schizophrenia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 31, 307326.Liddle, P.F., Morris, D.A., 1991. Schizophrenic syndromes and frontal lobe performance.

    The British Journal of Psychiatry 158, 340345.

    Moritz, S., Ruff, C., Wilke, U., Andresen, B., Krausz, M., Naber, D., 2001. Negative primingin schizophrenia: effects of masking and prime presentation time. SchizophreniaResearch 48, 291299.

    Park, S., Lenzenweger, M.F., Puschel, J., Holzman, P.S., 1996. Attentional inhibition inschizophrenia and schizotypy: a spatial negative priming study. CognitiveNeuropsychiatry 1 (2), 125149.

    Peters, E.R., Pickering, A.D., Hemsley, D.R., 1994. Cognitive inhibition and positivesymptomatology in schizotypy. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 33, 3348.

    Peters, E., Pickering, A., Kent, A., Glasper, A., Irani, M., Day, S., David, A., Hemsley, D.,2000. The relationship between cognitive inhibition and psychotic symptoms.

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 386395.Raine, A., 1991. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on

    DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin 17, 555564.Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Lencz, T., Scerbo, A., Triphon, N., Kim, D., 1991. Cognitiveperceptual,

    interpersonal, and disorganised features of schizotypal personality. SchizophreniaBulletin 20 (1), 191201.

    Ruiz, M.C.A., Barrantes-Vidal, N., Guitart, M., Fananas, L., 2008. Study of neurocognitivecorrelates of schizotypy personality clusters in healthy individuals. European Journalof Psychiatry 22 (1), 1728.

    Tipper, S., 1985. The negative priming effect: inhibitory priming by ignored objects.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 37A, 571590.

    Tsakanikos, E., Claridge, G., 2005. Less words, more words: verbal uency as a functionofpositive and negative schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences 39,705713.

    Wechsler, D., 1997. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III. Manual for Administrationand Scoring. David Wechsler Corporation. Harcourt Brace & Co, San Antonio.

    Williams, L.M., 1996. Cognitive inhibition and schizophrenic symptom subgroups.Schizophrenia Bulletin 22, 139151.

    234 M. Cochrane et al. / Psychiatry Research 196 (2012) 230234